59.4%United States United States
8.7%United Kingdom United Kingdom
5%Canada Canada
4%Australia Australia
3.5%Philippines Philippines
2.6%Netherlands Netherlands
2.4%India India
1.6%Germany Germany
1%France France
0.7%Poland Poland

Today: 189
Yesterday: 251
This Week: 189
Last Week: 2221
This Month: 4777
Last Month: 6796
Total: 129376

CHAPTER 9 GENERAL DISCUSSION

Books - Influence of Marijuana on Driving

Drug Abuse

CHAPTER 9 GENERAL DISCUSSION

9.1 Introduction

It is commonly assumed that cannabis smoking in real-life social situations delivers THC doses that seriously degrade the ability to safely operate motor vehicles; and, that the drug's users frequently drive shortly after smoking. If these premises are correct, it would follow that cannabis users are, while intoxicated, at increased risk of traffic accident involvement and constitute a safety hazard for other road users. However, the foremost impression one gains from reviewing the literature is that no clear relationship has ever been demonstrated between cannabis smoking and either seriously impaired driving performance or traffic safety. The epidemiological evidence, as limited as it is, shows that the combination of THC and alcohol is over-represented in injured and dead drivers and more so in those who actually caused the accidents. Yet there is little if any evidence to indicate that drivers who have used cannabis alone are any more likely to cause serious accidents than drug free drivers. To a large extent, the results from driving simulator and closed-course tests corroborate the epidemiological findings by indicating that THC in single inhaled doses up to about 250 Ag/kg has relatively minor effects on driving performance, certainly less than BACs in the range 0.08-0.10 g%.

But how well do these findings relate to the actual driving performance of regular cannabis users? If previous experience is any guide, little of crucial importance will emerge from experimental research until it is conducted in a more realistic manner. Therefore, a series of studies were conducted to determine the dose-response relationship between TI-IC and objectively and subjectively measured aspects of real world driving. A variety of driving tasks were employed, including maintenance of a constant speed and steady lateral position during uninterrupted highway travel, adjusting velocity to the movements of a leading car on a highway, and city driving. The purpose of applying different tests was to determine whether similar changes in performance under the influence of THC occurs in all thereby indicating a general drug effect on driving safety.

However real these tests might appear, the circumstances in which the subjects smoked the drug and drove the car were still somewhat artificial. First, the subjects consumed the drug in a neutral setting, alone or in the presence of a stranger. This was of course different from the situation wherein they normally smoke cannabis, i.e. in the company of friends who might reinforce a certain type of behavior not normally considered as conservative or prudent. Marks and Pow (1989) found that marijuana smokers derived slightly greater pleasure from THC in the company of friends than strangers from which one might infer a social amplification of the pharmacological effect. However, neither they nor any other investigators ever indicated that social amplification outlasts the situation which gives rise to it. In the absence of such evidence, we tend to discount the possibility that the unusual setting for drug administration had any effect upon subsequent driving performance. Social amplification of THC's adverse effects on driving performance might well occur if the driver were accompanied by similarly intoxicated passengers. We must therefore restrict generalizations from the studies' results to those situations where the intoxicated driver is doing his best to perform efficiently and is uninfluenced by friends or any other factor which might amplify the drug's adverse effects.

Secondly, subjects were aware of the fact that their driving behavior was being observed and that the accompanying driving instructor would intervene if necessary. The former is hardly avoidable in experimental studies but possibly provoked the subjects to do their utmost; the latter is a prerequisite for ensuring safety in actual driving studies and may or may not have produced nonchalance on the subjects' part resulting in poorer than normal performance. Yet according to what the subjects declared, they drove normally after placebo indicating that the experimental situation had not seriously altered their performance. Whether they drove in the test as they normally would after smoking cannabis remains open to question. The correspondence between experimental and epidemiological studies regarding THC's effects on driving performance suggests that this artificiality is also of minor concern.

The present series of studies are about the best simulation of real world driving one can reasonably achieve and have gone further toward defining the effects of cannabis smoking on actual driving performance than any of its predecessors. The results found in the successive studies were discussed at full length in the respective chapters. In this chapter, results of the separate studies will be integrated to provide answers to some important questions concerning marijuana's influence on driving performance. First however, the implications of the laboratory study's major result will be discussed, i.e. the THC dose that cannabis users prefer to achieve their desired 'high'. In the following section, the driving tests will be discussed in terms of the kinds of mental operations they require. Some further remarks provided in that section concern the relevance of the driving tests for traffic safety. The major part of this chapter will be addressed to the most important issue, i.e. what the results indicate as the real effects of marijuana smoking on driving performance. This chapter will end with a list of conclusions and recommendations.

9.2 THC Doses

To avoid arbitrarily selecting an unrealistic maximum THC dose for the driving studies, it was necessary to establish the dose that cannabis users actually prefer for achieving their desired 'high'. A laboratory study met this requirement. The study showed that cannabis users prefer higher THC doses than those previously administered in experimental studies. Previous THC doses were generally between 100 and 200 ig/kg, including the remaining butt, whereas the subjects in the laboratory study preferred an average dose of 308 Ag/kg, excluding the butt. This either means that today's cannabis users prefer higher doses than in the past or that investigators failed to administer realistic doses in previous studies, or both.

Two previous studies employed similar procedures as the present laboratory study to determine the THC dose that marijuana users prefer. Cappell etal. (1973) allowed subjects to smoke as many marijuana cigarettes, containing either 0.2, 0.4, or 0.8% THC, as they needed to achieve a 'nice high'. Comparing the weight of the remaining butt with the unlit cigarette revealed that the average THC consumption was inversely related to drug potency: with increasing potency, the twelve males participating in the study smoked 3.8, 6.0, and 11.0 mg THC, respectively. The investigators failed, however, to report the subjects' mean weight which makes it difficult to compare the results with those of the present study. Cappell and Pliner (1974) replicated the study with potencies of 0.36, 0.73, and 1.45%, and reported the average amounts of marijuana consumed by the sixty males participating in the study: they were 21.36, 15.57, and 13.34 mg/kg for the low, medium, and high potencies, respectively. Assuming that THC was equally distributed over the entire cigarettes, this means that the subjects smoked 77, 114, and 193 pg/kg THC, respectively. The potency of the cigarettes used in the present laboratory study was 2.57% and our subjects smoked on the average a THC dose of 308 Ag/kg. Assuming that the smoking technique did not change as a function of drug potency (for a discussion, see Section 2.5.2), these results suggest that the potency of the marijuana cigarette contributes to the preferred dose, at least if the potency is in the range of 0.2-2.5%. It is therefore hard to say whether current users indeed prefer higher doses than almost two decades ago or that they simply have been unable to sufficiently discriminate among the administered potencies. What is clear from the laboratory study's results, is that higher THC doses than those usually administered in previous studies should be included in future ones.

The relation between preferred dose and the potency of the drug raises two questions that might have implications for interpreting the present studies' results. The first question is, how do people regulate their THC consumption? Apparently they do not titrate brain THC concentrations to within a narrow range as tobacco smokers reliably do with nicotine (Herning et al., 1981; Gust and Pickens, 1982). It's also doubtful that marijuana smokers integrate their THC concentrations over time to cease consumption when the cumulative effect approaches a certain threshold. The probable explanation is that our subjects were somewhat aware of both the momentary brain concentration and its rate of increase and ceased smoking before the expected rise would shortly exceed the preferred effect. This procedure was accurate enough to limit the drug's effects to below what they had previously experienced as an unpleasant maximum. It was not accurate enough to attain homogeneity in the subjects' achievement of the preferred 'high' in relation to the maximum (coefficient of variation: 25%). In real life, cannabis users are usually more aware of the potency of the material they smoke, from prior experience, upon the advice of other users or even from its 'street' price. The material presented to the subjects in the laboratory study was definitely unfamiliar and they may have suddenly become prudent smokers. Confirmation of this supposition came from the same subjects' consumption of THC in the subsequent driving study. Though treated with the same average dose they had preferred in the laboratory study, their plasma drug concentrations were now higher. This difference was interpreted as the result of increased smoking efficiency due to familiarization with the potency of the material containing the drug.
If familiarization plays a major role in smoking efficiency and, consequently THc plasma concentrations, THC's adverse effects on performance may also be dependent on this factor. That being the case, the second and third driving studies' results would under-estimate

THC's adverse effects on driving performance since new groups of subjects were recruited in both. However, the remarkable resemblance between effects of every THC dose on SDLP in the first and second driving studies indicates that familiarization is not a very important factor. Nevertheless, we recommend that subjects in future studies be familiarized with the potency of the drug before the beginning of performance testing.

The second question raised by the apparent relationship between the drug's potency and the preferred dose is, what the latter would have been if marijuana of much higher potency were smoked. This is an important question since potencies of marijuana are increasing rapidly. At present, potencies of marijuana cultivated in The Netherlands may contain 5-15% THC. It is hard to imagine that subjects would consume as much of a cigarette containing a very high THC concentration as one containing a very low one. Future research determining marijuana users' preferred dose should also include marijuana with much higher potencies. It may then appear that the relationship between consumed THC dose and cigarette potency was attributable to the subjects' inability to discriminate among potencies spanning a relative narrow range. A broader range of cigarette potencies would probably allow regular marijuana users to discriminate well enough to alter their consumption and thereby come closer to administering the same preferred dose after every one.
On the basis of the laboratory study's results, the highest dose of THC administered in subsequent driving studies was defined as 300 Ag/kg; other doses were 100 and 200 Ag/kg, and placebo. Marijuana cigarettes were prepared from batches with potencies varying from 1.75% to 3.58%, the highest that were then available. If future research shows that current users of marijuana prefer much higher THC doses when they smoke very potent marijuana, the results of the current program can only serve as an indication of the minimal effects the drug may have on actual driving performance. If, on the other hand, it shows that users prefer a THC dose of about 300 itg/kg to reach their desired 'high' irrespective of the potency of the drug, the results presented in this report are truly valid estimates of what may happen in daily life after smoking THC doses that induce a preferred 'high'.

9.3 The Driving Tests

Three different tests were applied in the experiments that progressively increased in the number of skills employed by the driver: road tracking, car following and city driving. The first is now a standardized test which, in more than 40 studies, has proven to be very sensitive to sedation produced by a variety of medicinal and social drugs, including alcohol. The car following and city driving test were developed following the recognition that parameters measured in the standard test fail to represent all abilities considered important for safe driving. This section will describe the differences between the driving tests and the relevance of each to traffic safety.

The difference between the three driving tests can be characterized in many ways. The one described here is that in terms of the type of information processing each requires. Two distinct types of human information processing can be distinguished (Shiffrin and Schneider, 1977): 'automatic' and 'controlled'. The former is capable of accepting an enormous volume of perceptual information at a relatively high rate to mediate coordinated multi-effector responding in a normally stable input-output relationship. This process is not generally under voluntary control, proceeds in parallel with the stream of consciousness and involves no decision making. Controlled information processing is much slower but highly adaptive. It begins with the conscious perception of a discrete event or situation. Identification of the meaning of that perception is made by comparing it with information stored in memory. A decision involving the selection and execution of a particular series of motor responses and/or the suppression of a motor routine in progress occurs next. This process can proceed in parallel with an automatic process and the integrity of both are essential for safe driving.

Aspects of both types of information processing are present in most well practiced tasks. Most would agree that skilled performance is the integrated sum of automatic and controlled information processing and that their respective weights vary constantly with the task requirements. The more controlled information processing a task requires, the more it is experienced as demanding effort. According to this concept, lateral position control in the road tracking test depends principally upon automatic information processing. The car following test requires somewhat more controlled information processing and driving in urban traffic, even more. The concept can be illustrated by the ease of having a conversation with other occupants of a vehicle while driving. It is easy to converse with them while driving on a highway in light traffic. It becomes somewhat more difficult when driving in heavy traffic and maintaining a safe headway behind cars travelling in a platoon. Conversing while driving in urban traffic is even more difficult and, at times, even impossible for most drivers. Thus, the more controlled processing involved in a particular situation, the more effort the driving task demands and the less 'spare capacity' is left for having a conversation with other occupants. This concept of effort relates to task or computational demands, and often, to the allocation of attention. There exists, however, also another concept of effort which relates to brain arousal mechanisms (Kahneman, 1973; Pribram and McGuinness, 1975) or the required psychological state (Hockey, 1986). If one perceives a discrepancy between his actual state of arousal and that required to efficiently accomplish the task, he will first attempt to resolve the difference by compensatory effort, e.g. focussing attention. Failing that, he will try to reduce task demands to what can be efficiently accomplished in the deficient arousal state. The latter can be accomplished during driving by reducing speed or assuming a greater headway to compensate for slower reaction times.

It is sensible to keep the relevance of the separate driving tests for driving and traffic safety in mind while discussing THC's effects on driving performance. It is obvious that the road tracking test measures only a few aspects of driving behavior. Yet proper road tracking is a general prerequisite for safe driving. Consequently, drugs that substantially impair a driver's fundamental road tracking ability possess a real potential for adversely affecting driving safety. Whether this means that drivers under the influence of such drugs have also a greater probability of becoming involved in a traffic accident seems plausible but has yet to be determined. The same kind of reasoning applies to the car following test. It measures only a few additional aspects of driving behavior, but their impairment is also incompatible with safe driving. The city driving test measures most aspects of driving behavior and therefore comes closest to reality. Driving in urban traffic is so common in daily life that any drug-induced impairment found with this test should be considered as the most important, though not necessarily the earliest, sign that the drug possesses properties that adversely affect traffic safety.

9.4    Effects of THC on Driving Performance

9.4.1    Results of Present Studies

One of the issues addressed by the first driving study was whether it would be safe to continue using the same approach for subsequent on-road studies in traffic. The first group complied with all instructions, even after high doses of THC. Changes in mood were often reported but the subjects were always able to complete every ride without major interventions by the driving instructors and their safety was never compromised. The same occurred in the subsequent studies showing that it is possible to safely study marijuana's effects on actual driving performance in the presence of other traffic. In this respect, the drug is no different from many others studied by the same investigators and their colleagues.

The standard test measured the subjects' ability to maintain a constant speed and a steady lateral position between the lane boundaries. Standard deviation of lateral position, SDLP, increased after marijuana smoking in a dose-related manner. The lowest dose, i.e. 100 mg/kg THC, produced a slight elevation in mean SDLP, albeit significant in the first driving study. The intermediate dose, i.e. 200 Ag/kg THC, increased SDLP moderately; and, the highest, i.e. 300 Ag/kg THC, substantially. It is remarkable how well the changes in SDLP following THC in the first driving study were replicated in the second, in spite of the many differences in the ways they were designed. The replication of THc's effects on SDLP substantiates the generality of these results. Other objective measures obtained by this test were much less affected by THC. Mean speed was somewhat reduced following the higher THC doses, but the effects were relatively small (max. 1.1 km/h or 0.7 mph). Standard deviations of speed and steering wheel movements were unaffected by the drug. Subjective ratings of perceived driving quality followed a similar pattern as SDLP indicating that the subjects were well aware of their diminished ability to control the vehicle after marijuana smoking.

The car following test measured the subjects' ability to follow a leading car with varying speed at a constant distance. All THC doses increased mean headway, but according to an inverse dose-response relationship. This type of relationship was unexpected and probably due to the particular design of the second driving study, i.e. the ascending dose series. It means that subjects were very cautious the first time they undertook the test under the influence of THC (i.e. after the lowest dose) and progressively less thereafter. As a consequence of this phenomenon, mean reaction time to changes in the preceding car's speed also followed an inverse dose-response relationship. Statistical adjustment for this confounding by analysis of covariance indicated that reaction times would not have increased significantly if the mean headway were constant. Coefficient of headway variation increased slightly following THC. Together, these data indicate that there is no more than a slight tendency towards impairment in car following performance after marijuana smoking. They also show that subjects try to compensate for anticipated adverse effects of the drug by increasing headway, especially when they are uncertain of what these might be. As in the standard test, subjects' ratings of driving quality corresponded to the objective changes in their performance.
The city driving study measured the subjects' ability to operate a vehicle in urban traffic. For reasons mentioned in the respective chapter the THC dose in that study was restricted to 100 fig/kg. For comparative purposes another group of subjects was treated with a modest dose of alcohol, producing a mean BAG of about 0.04 g%. Results of the study showed that the modest dose of alcohol, but not THC, produced a significant impairment in driving performance, relative to placebo. Alcohol impaired driving performance but subjects did not perceive it. THC did not impair driving performance yet the subjects thought it had. After alcohol, there was a tendency towards faster driving and after THC, slower.

The results of these studies corroborate those of previous driving simulator and closed-course tests by indicating that THC in single inhaled doses up to 300 Ag/kg has significant, yet not dramatic, dose-related impairing effects on driving performance. They contrast with results from many laboratory tests, reviewed by Moskowitz (1985), which show that even low doses of THC impair skills deemed important for driving, such as perception, coordination, tracking and vigilance. The present studies also demonstrated that marijuana can have greater effects in laboratory than driving tests. The last study, for example, showed a highly significant effect of THC on hand unsteadiness but not on driving in urban traffic.

9.4.2 Drug Plasma Concentrations and Driving Performance

One of the program's objectives was to determine whether it is possible to predict driving impairment by plasma concentrations of THC and/or its metabolite, THC-COOH, in single samples. The answer is very clear: it is not. Plasma of drivers showing substantial impairment in these studies contained both high and low THC concentrations; and, drivers with high plasma concentrations showed substantial, but also no impairment, or even some improvement. The first driving study showed that impairment in the road tracking test was nearly the same in the first and second test, executed between 40-60 and 100-120 minutes after initiation of smoking, respectively. Plasma concentrations of THC and THC-COOH, however, were not the same during the tests: both were lower during the second than the first. The same pattern was found for ratings of perceived 'high'. It has been said that behavioral signs of intoxication, though small, outlast physiological and subjective reactions to THC (Reeve et al., 1983; Yesavage et al., 1985). To examine this hypothesis, future research should extend actual driving performance measurements to 4, 8, lb and 24 hours after smoking. If driving impairment still occurs after THC disappears from plasma, it could mean that previous epidemiological research has underestimated the proportion of drivers who were driving under the influence of cannabis at the times their accidents occurred.

Mean speed was the only measure of driving performance that was even moderately related to plasma concentrations of the drug. Subjects with higher THC concentrations in plasma drove slower in the standard road tracking test (correlations varying from r = -.18 to r=-.72 between conditions). This effect might have been even more pronounced if the subjects had not been instructed to drive at a particular speed, and if they had had no feedback from the speedometer.

9.4.3 Cannabis versus Alcohol and Other Psychotropic Drugs

How do marijuana's effects on driving performance compare to those of alcohol? There are two sources from which one can draw to answer the question. Information can be directly obtained from studies comparing THC and alcohol effects in the same experiment; and, indirectly, from studies wherein alcohol's effects were assessed using the same methods as applied in the present THC studies. As mentioned in Chapter 3, most closed-course studies on THC also measured alcohol's effects (BAcs between 0.04 and 0.10 g%). It was generally concluded that THC's effects were less than alcohol's, especially at BACs above 0.08 g%. The city driving study in the present program also compared the effects of modest doses of alcohol and THC. For doses administered in that study, alcohol produced the greater effects. Indirect evidence concerning the relative effects of THC and alcohol can be obtained from three studies. First, the alcohol calibration study by Louwerens et aL (1985, 1987) which resembled our first driving study in many respects. According to their empirical equation, THC's effects on SDLP were equal to or less than that of BAC =0.07 g%. More recently, studies by Riedel et al. (1989) and Ramaekers et al. (1992a) measured the effects of low doses of alcohol (BAEs of 0.05 and 0.03 g% respectively) on SDLP. Both groups applied the standard test in the presence of other traffic, as in our second driving study, but on another highway. Mean SDLPs were respectively about 5.0 and 2.5 cm higher while driving after alcohol than placebo. The former elevation is greater than that produced by the highest THC dose in our study. The latter lies between the effects of 200 and 300 gig/kg doses, which were 1.8 and 2.9 cm respectively. There was some discrepancy between alcohol's effects on SDLP in the more recent studies and those predicted by the empirical equation: the former were higher than predicted. The discrepancy appears to be related to the difference between alcohol's effects on the ascending and descending phases of its pharmacokinetic profile. Louwerens measured alcohol's effects at the time when BAG was at the ascending but Riedel and Ramaekers measured them during the descending phase. Notwithstanding methodological differences among studies, both direct and indirect evidence converge on the conclusion that THC's effects after doses up to 300 jig/kg never exceed alcohol's at BAGS of 0.08 eh.

How do marijuana's effects on driving performance compare to those of drugs other than alcohol? No direct comparisons have ever been made, but many studies employing the standard road tracking test were conducted for measuring other drugs' effects on SDLP during the last decade. The results from a few will be mentioned. Note that THC's greatests effects on SDLP were 3.7 and 2.9 cm in our first and second driving study, respectively. Diazepam (Valium') given for one week in a low therapeutic dose (5 mg, thrice daily) caused anxious patients to drive with a mean SDLP about 7 cm higher than their premedication baseline (Van Laar et al., 1992). The same drug and dose given over the same period caused healthy volunteers to drive with a mean SDLP about 6 cm higher than placebo (Van Veggel and O'Hanlon, 1993). Lorazepam (Ativan), another anxiolytic, given twice daily for one week in a 1.5 mg dose to healthy volunteers (Volkerts et aL, 1988) and a 2 mg dose to patients (Vermeeren et al., 1993), produced an elevation of SDLP of about 10 cm in both cases. Amitriptyline (Flavin, a widely prescribed antidepressant, given in a dose of 50 mg at night and 25 mg in the morning caused healthy volunteers to drive with a mean SDLP about 6 cm higher than placebo (Robbe et al., 1989). Flurazepam (Dalmane), a hypnotic, was administered to insomniacs and its 'hang-over' effects on SDLP were measured 10-11 hours after ingestion. A 15 mg dose of flurazepam elevated mean SDLP by about 4 cm; a 30 mg dose, 7 cm. Antihistamines also cause sedation and, consequently, impair road tracking performance. Triprolidine (Actifecr) increased SDLP by 3.5 cm after a single 5 mg dose (Riedel et al., 1990); and, diphenhydramine 50 mg (Benadryl Kapseals') increased SDLP by 4.5 cm (Ramaekers et al., 1992b). This is not to say that all psychotropic drugs produce greater elevations of SDLP than THC. Many in the same and other experiments had less effect than THC did in our studies. These examples are merely cited to indicate that THC's effects as measured in the standard test were in no way unusual. In so far as its effects on SDLP are concerned, THC was just another moderately impairing drug.

The foregoing comparisons might be misleading. THC's effects differ qualitatively from many other drugs, especially alcohol. For example, subjects drive faster after drinking alcohol and slower after smoking marijuana (Hansteen et aL, 1976; Casswell, 1979; Peck et al., 1986; Smiley et aL, 1987). Moreover, the simulator study by Ellingstad et al. (1973) showed that subjects under the influence of marijuana were less likely to engage in overtaking maneuvers, whereas those under the influence of alcohol showed the opposite tendency. Very importantly, our city driving study showed that drivers who drank alcohol overestimated their performance quality whereas those who smoked marijuana underestimated it. Perhaps as a consequence, the former invested no special effort for accomplishing the task whereas the latter did, and successfully. This evidence strongly suggests that alcohol encourages risky driving whereas THC encourages greater caution, at least in experiments. Another way THC seems to differ qualitatively from many other drugs is that the former's users seem better able to compensate for its adverse effects while driving under the influence. Weil et aL (1968) were among the earliest authors who mentioned the possibility that marijuana users can actively suppress the drug's adverse effects. They presumed that THC's effects were confined to higher cortical functions without any general stimulatory or depressive effect on lower brain centers. According to them, the relative absence of neurological, as opposed to psychiatric, symptoms in marijuana intoxication suggests this possibility. More recently, Moskowitz (1985) concluded that the variety of impairments found after marijuana smoking could not be explained by decrements in sensory or motor functions which led him to hypothesize that some important central cognitive process is impaired by THC, without saying what it is. The recent discovery of abundant cannabinoid receptor concentrations in the cerebral cortex and the hippocampal formation corroborates these hypotheses and, with other findings to come, will certainly greatly enhance our understanding of the drug's psychopharmacological effects.

9.4.4 Why are THc's Effects on Driving Performance relatively Small?

It is a natural question why the effects of marijuana on actual driving performance appear to be so small. As in many previous investigations, subjects attempted to compensate for anticipated adverse effects of marijuana smoking. Our subjects were aware of the impairing effects of THC as shown by lower ratings of perceived driving quality. Consequently, they invested more effort to accomplish the driving tests following THC than placebo. Furthermore, in the car following test, they drove at a greater headway after marijuana smoking; and, in both road tracking and city driving tests, they slightly reduced their driving speed. Yet despite their effort, subjects were unable to fully compensate for THC's adverse effects on lateral position variability. This is because SDLP is primarily controlled by an automatic information processing system which operates outside of conscious control. The process is relatively impervious to environmental changes, as shown by the high reliability of SDLP under repeated placebo conditions, but highly vulnerable to internal factors that retard the flow of information through the system. THC and many other drugs are among these factors. When they interfere with the process that restricts SDLP, there is little the afflicted individual can do by way of compensation to restore the situation. Car following and, to a greater extent, city driving performance depend more on controlled information processing and are therefore more accessible for compensatory mechanisms that reduce the decrements or abolish them entirely.

This still leaves the question open why performance appears to be more affected by THC in laboratory than actual driving tests. Many researchers defend the primacy of laboratory performance tests for measuring drug effects on skills related to driving on the basis of superior experimental control. Certainly some control is always necessary to reduce the confounding influence of extraneous factors that would otherwise so increase measurement error as to totally obscure the drug's effects. However, only some extraneous factors are truly sources of measurement error and others either attenuate or amplify drug effects in real driving and must be considered as relevant to a test's predictive validity. Simply eliminating all of them, first, removes their normal mediating influence on the drug effect, and secondly, affects the subject's motivation to perform the test by making it appear 'unreal'. Controlling the test usually involves drastic simplification and restriction of response options. The desire in doing this is to isolate a particular driving skill and determine how it changes under the influence of drugs. However, drivers always apply numerous skills in parallel and series. Should one become deficient, they are often able to compensate in a number of ways to achieve a satisfactory level of proficiency. Thus the demonstration of some particular skill decrement in the laboratory in no way indicates that this would ultimately reduce driving safety in reality. Finally there are some skills that simply can not be measured in laboratory tests, at least not easily enough to make it a routine matter. The acquisition of any skill which depends upon automatic information processing requires practice over weeks or months. After learning to drive, subjects possess such skills in abundance and one can only demonstrate how they vary with drug effects in the real task or a very close approximation thereof.

Profound drug impairment constituting an obvious traffic safety hazard could as easily be demonstrated in a laboratory performance test as anywhere else. But THC is not a profoundly impairing drug. It does affect automatic information processing, even after low doses, but not to any great extent after high doses. It apparently affects controlled information processing in a variety of laboratory tests, but not to the extent which is beyond the individual's ability to control when he is motivated and permitted to do so in real driving. In short, it would appear as if over-control in laboratory performance tests has resulted in a misimpression of THC's effect, incomplete in some respects and exaggerated in others. The actual driving tests may provide a more realistic impression of the drug's effects, albeit still incomplete and perhaps tending to minimize them with respect to more complex driving situations that come closer to 'worst case'.

The degree of experimental control also varied between driving tests in this series in ways affecting the subjects' motivation. This is illustrated by a comparison between the first and second driving study. The standard road tracking test was applied in both, first in the absence and then in the presence of other traffic. It was only during the former that disturbing observations of two individuals' attentional deficits caused the driving instructor to intervene. Driving in the presence of other traffic, subjects were always able to complete the rides without intervention. Lateral position control, an automatic process, did not change as a consequence of the absence or presence of other traffic. What did change was the subjects' motivation to focus attention, a controlled process. Motivation in the second study was very probably affected by recognition of the increased risk of the untoward consequences of wandering attention. This means that the intrinsic motivation produced by the reality of the test situation is an important mediator of THC's effects on performance.

Compensatory mechanisms help the driver under the influence of marijuana to maintain an effective level of performance but with an associated cost. If drivers compensate for THC's adverse effects by diminishing driving demands (e.g. by reducing speed and/or increasing headway), this will occur without a reduction in spare capacity. But if they increase effort as well (e.g. by focusing attention), it will occur at the expense of spare capacity. Less capacity would be left for simultaneously performing another task, such as conversing with passengers, using a car telephone, or handling emergency situations. The information processing capacity these situations demand may well go beyond the driver's spare capacity with the result of impaired and perhaps dangerous driving. Results of the present program show that THC increases the mental load of driving, as shown by increased effort ratings and reduced heart rate variability, and consequently reduces spare capacity. This corroborates results from previous simulator and closed-course studies that with reasonable consistency show an adverse THC effect on subsidiary task performance (Smiley, 1986). Further research is required to determine marijuana's effects on actual driving performance when the driver is simultaneously performing another task or suddenly confronted with a situation that requires a rapid adaptive response. The latter was occasionally encountered during the city driving test, but only after a low THC dose. The city driving test should therefore be repeated with subjects consuming higher THC doses.

Hazardous driving can also occur in situations that demand very little of the driver's information procesing capacity. If the driving task is very monotonous and the demand is low, wandering attention may result in negligent monitoring with disastrous results. This is in fact what happened twice during the driving study on the closed road. After the highest THC dose, one subject failed to shift attention from the prescribed task to an unexpected event (screwdriver on the road); another failed to anticipate a normal event (end of circuit). Though even sober experienced drivers may experience similar deficits, the fact that it happened twice after the highest THC dose, and never after a lower dose or placebo, strongly suggests that drivers under the influence of THC would be unusually susceptible to attentional deficits during prolonged and monotonous driving.

9.4.5 Concluding Remarks

Epidemiological research has shown that THC is infrequently detected in the blood of fatally injured drivers as the only drug present. In most cases alcohol is also detected. The effects of the combination of THC and alcohol on actual driving performance have never been studied in the presence of other traffic. Closed-course studies have shown that the effects of both drugs, when taken in combination, are generally additive (Attwood et al., 1981; Peck et al., 1986) . This may only be so for those behaviors that are similarly affected by both drugs given separately. Closer examination of the combined use is warranted in those driving situations where both drugs produce qualitatively different effects. It may well be so that alcohol reduces drivers' insight or motivation to the point where they would no longer attempt to compensate for the THC effect. As a result, the combined effects on drivers' performance could well be greater than the sum of either drug acting separately. There is therefore a great need for further research on marijuana and actual driving research, but now extended to the combination of marijuana and alcohol.

In summary, this program of research has shown that marijuana, when taken alone, produces a moderate degree of driving impairment which is related to the consumed THC dose. The impairment manifests itself mainly in the ability to maintain a steady lateral position on the road, but its magnitude is not exceptional in comparison with changes produced by many medicinal drugs and alcohol. Drivers under the influence of marijuana retain insight in their performance and will compensate where they can, for example, by slowing down or increasing effort. As a consequence, THC's adverse effects on driving performance appear relatively small. The methods used for demonstrating THC's influence on driving performance were the same as for showing driving impairment after other drugs. That is their strength and weakness. In so far as THC's effects resemble those of other psychoactive drugs, their effects in the same tests can be compared. To the extent that THC possesses other effects not shown in these tests, the chance increases that something of importance was overlooked. One can easily imagine situations where the influence of marijuana smoking might have an exceedingly dangerous effect; i.e., emergency situations which put high demands on the driver's information processing capacity, prolonged monotonous driving, and after THC has been taken with other drugs, especially alcohol. Because these possibilities are real, this dissertation should not be considered as the final word. It should, however, remain for a while as the point of departure for subsequent studies that will ultimately complete the picture of THC's effects on driving performance.

We therefore agree with Moskowitz' (1985) conclusion that "any situation in which safety both for self and others depends upon alertness and capability of control of man-machine interaction precludes the use of marihuana (p. 342)." However, the magnitude of marijuana's, relative to many other drugs', effects also justify Gieringer's (1988) conclusion that "marijuana impairment presents a real, but secondary, safety risk; and that alcohol is the leading drug-related accident risk factor (p. 100)." Of the many psychotropic drugs, licit and illicit, that are available and used by people who subsequently drive, cannabis may well be among the least harmful. Campaigns to discourage the use of cannabis by drivers are certainly warranted. But concentrating a campaign on cannabis alone may not be in proportion to the safety problem it causes.

 

Show Other Articles Of This Author